Spiritual Intelligence

March 21, 2019 | Author: International Journal of Transpersonal Studies | Category: Spirituality, Intelligence, Consciousness, Factor Analysis, Behavioural Sciences
Share Embed Donate


Short Description

Download Spiritual Intelligence...

Description

 A Viable Model and and Sel-Report Measure Measure o Spiritual Intelligence David B. King

eresa L. DeCicco

rent University  Peterborough, ON, Canada   A our-actor model o spiritual spiritual intelligence is rst proposed. Supportive evidence is is reviewed or or the capacities o critical existential thinking, personal meaning production, transcendental awareness, and conscious state expansion. Based on this model, a 24-item sel-report measure was developed and modied across two consecutive studies (N = 619 and N = 304, respectively). Te nal version o the scale, the Spiritual Intelligence Sel-Report Inventory (SISRI-24), displayed excellent internal reliability and good t to the proposed model. Correlational analyses with additional measures o  meaning, metapersonal sel-construal, mysticism, religiosity, and social desirability oer support or construct and criterion-related validity. According to both intelligence criteria and current psychometric standards, ndings validate the proposed model and measure o spiritual intelligence. Future directions are discussed.



he nature o human intelligence and its psychological study st udy have been areas o continuous scientic debate (or a review, see Cianciolo & Sternberg, 2004). Many have argued that the sum o  human intelligence is best described as a single construct, constr uct, such as the intelligence quotient (IQ), while others have suggested multiple intelligences (Cianciolo & Sternberg, 2004; Gardner, 1983; Sternberg, 1988). Howard Gardner, a leading advocate o the latter standpoint, has proposed eight intelligences, including linguistic, logical-mathematical, spatial, musical, naturalist, and bodily-kinesthetic (Gardner, 1983, 1993, 1999). Recent decades have also witnessed extensive literature on social and emotional intelligences, which describe cognitive abilities o emotional perception and management on intra- and inter-personal levels (e.g., Gardner, 1983; Goleman, 1995; Mayer, Caruso, & Salovey, 2000; Salovey & Mayer, 1990). In order to evaluate potential additional intelligences (e.g., moral intelligence; Gardner, 1993), leading theorists have suggested rigid criteria that must rst be satised. It is generally established that an intelligence should (1) include a set o moderately  interrelated mental abilities (i.e., core capacities or which cognition is primary; those which are distinct rom preerred behaviors or traits), (2) acilitate adaptation,

68

problem-solving, and reasoning in all environmental contexts, and (3) develop with age and experience (Gardner, 1983; Mayer et al., 2000; Sternberg, 1997). Gardner (1983) also recommended neurological/ biological evidence, evolutionary e volutionary plausibility, and support support rom psychometrics and experimental psychology. Spiritual Intelligen I ntelligence ce  the additional intelligences proposed, the t he concept o spiritual intelligence has remained a orerunner in the past decade (Amram, 2007; Emmons, 2000a; Nasel, 2004; Noble, 2000; Vaughan, 2002; Wolman, 2001; Zohar & Marshall, 2000). Emmons (2000a) provided support or spiritual intelligence according to Gardner’s (1983) criteria, proposing ve core abilities: 1) the capacity or transcendent awareness (o a divine being or onesel); 2) the ability to enter spiritual states o consciousness; 3) the ability to sanctiy everyday  experiences; 4) the ability to utilize spirituality to solve problems; and 5) the capacity to engage in virtuous behaviors (e.g., orgiveness). Te last o these capacities has since been removed (Emmons, 2000b) due to its more accurate interpretation as preerred behavior (Mayer, 2000). Noble (2000) concurred with Emmons’ (2000a) conception o spiritual intelligence and added two additional core abilities: (1) “the conscious recognition

O

International Journal of ranspersonal Studies , 28 , 2009, pp. 68-85

King & DeCicco

that physical reality is embedded within a larger, multidimensional multidimensio nal reality” rea lity” (p. 46); and a nd (2) “the conscious pursuit o psychological health, not only or ourselves but or…the global community” (p. 46). Te rst o these abilities can be readily amalgamated with Emmons’ (2000a) capacity or transcendent awareness, while the second more closely c losely resembles preerred behavior. Te capacity or transcendent awareness has been equally emphasized by Wolman (2001) and Vaughan (2002). Others have added the capacity or existential thinking and questioning (e.g., Nasel, 2004; Vaughan, 2002; Wolman, 2001; Zohar & Marshall, 2000) as a  core aspect o spiritual intelligence. Zohar and Marshall (2000) urther contended that spiritual intelligence represents the brain’s unitive processes which serve to reconceptualize experience and produce meaning. Nasel (2004) described the construct as “the application o  spiritual abilities and resources to practical contexts” (p. 4), identiying two main components o existential questioning and the awareness o divine presence. His 1717item Spiritual Intelligence Scale incorporates traditional Christian values and New Age spirituality, yet ails to oer a universal measure o spiritual abilities as opposed to experiences and behaviors. More recently, Amram (2007) identied seven major themes o spiritual intelligence, including  meaning, consciousness, grace, transcendence, truth, peaceul surrender to Sel, and inner-directed reedom. His Integrated Spiritual Intelligence Scale consists o  22 subscales organized into ve theoretical domains o  consciousness, grace, meaning, transcendence, and truth (Amram & Dryer, 2007). Like Nasel (2004), however,  Amram (2007) ailed to distinguish careul careully ly among  spiritual ability, behavior, and experience, resulting in a model which is best described as a lived spirituality. Previous models have made similar errors. For example,  Wolman  Wo lman (200 (2001 1) contend contended ed that pheno phenomenolo menological gical experience is a critical component o spiritual intelligence,  while others have involved theological interpretatio interpretations ns (e.g.,, Emmons, 2000a; Nasel, 2004), resulting in limited (e.g. theories which cannot be universally universal ly applied. Gardner (1993, 2000) has remained hesitant to accept a spiritual intelligence in his own model o  multiple intelligences, arguing that the concept is too conounded with phenomenological experience and religious belie. He has, however however,, expressed preeren preerence ce or an existential intelligence, which Halama and Strizenec (2004) described as a related and overlapping construct to spiritual intelligence. Te current paper will oer

evidence to support the notion that spiritual intelligence not only involves involves existential exi stential capacities, but that it exists as a set o mental abilities that are distinct rom behavioral traits and experiences, satisying established intelligence criteria (Gardner, 1983; Mayer et al., 2000; Sternberg, 1997). Te subsequent model also assumes a  contemporary interpretation o spirituality that distinguishes it rom the construct o religiosity (King, Speck, & Tomas, 2001; Koenig, McCullough, & Larson, 2000; Love, 2002; 20 02; Sinnott, 2002; Wink & Dillon, 2002;  Wul, 1991 1991). ). Religion is viewed as “an organized system o belies, practices, rituals, and symbols” (Koenig et al., 2000, p. 18), 18), while spirituality is regarded as a s “the personal quest or understanding answers to ultimate questions about lie, about meaning, and about relationship to the sacred or transcendent” (p. 18). As many authors (e.g., Helminiak, 2001; Worthington & Sandage, 2001) have noted, however, however, religion and a nd spirituality remain intimately  connected, with religion being “the social vehicle that, at its best, proclaims and supports spirituality” (Helminiak, 2001, p. 165). While religion and spiritual intelligence are also likely related, this is a topic o discussion which cannot be adequately addressed within the connes o  the current paper. Nevertheless, it is maintained that spiritual intelligence and religiosity are distinct but related psychological constructs. As such, the current model may or may not be consistent with established religious approaches or systems o belie. Tis is not a  setback o the current model; rather, it is the result o  a commitment to the identication o cognitive abilities as opposed to belies and attitudes, which is necessary  in the establishment o a universal human intelligence (Gardner, 1983; Mayer et al., 2000; Sternberg, 1997).  A Viable Model o o Spiritual Intelligence Intell igence n the current model, spiritual intelligence is dened as a set o mental capacities which contribute to the awareness, integration, and adaptive application o the nonmaterial and transcendent aspects o one’s existence, leading to such outcomes as deep existential reection, enhancement o meaning, recognition o a transcendent sel, and mastery o spiritual states (King, 2008). An extensive review o the literature supports our core components: componen ts: (1) (1) critical existential thinking, thi nking, (2) personal meaning production, (3) transcendental awareness, and (4) conscious state expansion. Following a discussion o  each o these capacities, additional support or adaptive applications and development over the liespan will be reviewed.

Spiritual Intelligence

International Journal of ranspersonal Studies  69

I

Critical Existential Tinking (CE) Te rst component o spiritual intelligence involves the capacity to critically contemplate meaning, purpose, and other existential or metaphysical issues (e.g., reality, the universe, space, time, death). In addition to the mounting support or Gardner’s (1993) suggestion o an existential intelligence (e.g., Halama  & Strizenec, 2004; Shearer, 2006; Simmons, 2006), existential thinking is commonplace in denitions o  both spirituality (e.g., Koenig, McCullough, & Larson, 2000; Matheis, ulsky, & Matheis, 2006; Wink & Dillon, 2002) and spiritual intelligence (Nasel, 2004; Vaughan, 2002; Wolman, 2001; Zohar & Marshall, 2000). It is currently argued that critical existential thinking can be applied to any lie issue, as any object or event can be viewed in relation to one’s existence.  While some discuss disc uss a “quest “quest or understanding answers” ans wers” (Koenig et al., 2000, p. 18) to these seemingly ultimate questions (e.g., Noble, 2000), this can more practically  be considered as a related pattern o behavior. o a large extent, aspects o cognition are inherent in the discussion o existential tendencies, with requent reerences to existential thinking (e.g., Garo, 2006), existential exi stential contemplation (e.g., Lavoie & de Vries, 2004), and existential reasoning rea soning (e.g., Evans & Wellman, Wellman, 2006), leading to the more plausible inerence o mental capacity. Furthermore, sel-estimates o intelligence have revealed that perceived existential intelligence is a signicant predictor o perceived overall intelligence (Furnham, Wytykowska, & Petrides, 2002). It is urther contended that simply questioning  existence does not demonstrate complete mastery o this ability. One must be able to contemplate such exis tential issues using critical thinking, and in some cases come to original conclusions or personal philosophies regarding  existence, integrating scientic knowledge and personal experience. Critical thinking, dened as “actively  and skilully conceptualizing, applying, analyzing, synthesizing, and/or evaluating inormation gathered rom, or generated by, observation, experience, reect ion, reasoning, or communication” (Scriven & Paul, 1992), more accurately reects intelligence. Personal Meaning Production (PMP) Te second core component is dened as the ability to construct personal meaning and purpose in all physical and mental experiences, including the capacity  to create and master a lie purpose. Like existential thinking, personal meaning is requently described as a  component o spirituality (e.g., King, Speck, & Tomas,

2001; Koenig et al., 2001; a l., 2000; Sinnott, 2002; Wink & Dillon, 2002; Worthington & Sandage, 2001), requiring its consideration in a model o spiritual intelligence. Nasel (2004) concurred, suggesting that spiritual intelligence “involves contemplation o the symbolic meaning o  personal events and circumstances, in order to nd purpose and meaning in all lie experiences” (p. 52). In essence, Emmons’ (2000a) capacity or sanctication is one particular method o personal meaning production. Personal meaning has been dened as “having  a purpose in lie, having a sense o direction, a sense o  order and a reason or existence” (Reker, 1997, p. 710). Meddin (1998) identied a cognitive component o  personal meaning, dened as “an integrative organizing  principle (or set o principles) which enables one to make sense (cognition) o one’s inner lie and outer environment” (p. 164), which closely resembles Zohar and Marshall’s (2000) description o the brain’s tertiary  processes. A cognitive component was also suggested by Wong (1989), who dened personal meaning as “an individually constructed cognitive system, that is… capable o endowing lie with personal signicance and satisaction” (p. 517). Reker’s (1997) denition highlights the undamental relationship between meaning and purpose. In addition to deriving purpose rom daily  events and experiences (i.e., situational meaning), one is also capable o dening a purpose or his/her lie (i.e., global meaning), utilizing more coherent and creative orms o meaning production. Te mastery o a lie purpose reers to one’s ability to iner his/her purpose in all events and experiences. A seemingly innite number o sources o meaning and purpose have been described in the literature, including leisure activities (Reker &  Wong,  Wo ng, 1988) 988),, work (Tompson, 1992) 992),, reminiscence (Wong, 1989), and dreams (aylor, 2001), to name a  ew. Te ability to create meaning and purpose in all mental and physical experiences occupies the highest level o this particular ability. ranscendental Awareness (A) Te third component involves the capacity  to perceive transcendent dimensions o the sel (e.g., a  transcendent sel), o others, and o the physical world (e.g., nonmaterialism, interconnectedness) during the normal, waking state o consciousness. Te transc endent is commonplace in denitions o spirituality (e.g., King et al., 2001; Koenig et al., 2000; 200 0; Martsol Mart sol & Mickley, Mick ley, 1998; 1998; Sinnott, 2002), with one aspect o particular interest to the current model: awareness (Martsol & Mickley,

International Journal of ranspersonal Studies 

King & DeCicco

70

1988). Pascual (1990) proposed that transcendental awareness, particularly the awareness o a transcenden transcendental tal sel, is a key actor o spiritual intelligence. Wolman (2001) explained this more broadly as the ability to sense a spiritual dimension o lie, reecting what  James (1 (1902/2002) 902/2002) had previously described as “a sense o present reality more diused and general than that  which our specia speciall senses yield” (p. 72) 72).. Other authors have concurred (e.g., Emmons, 2000a; Nasel, 2004; Noble; 2001). Building on the work o Abraham Maslow, Hamel, Leclerc, Leclerc , and Lerançois (2003) have described an additional addition al process o transcenden transcendent-actualizatio t-actualization, n, which they dene as “a sel-realization sel-realizat ion ounded on an awareness and experience o a Spiritual Center, also called calle d the Inner Being or the Sel” (p. 4). Csikszentmihalyi (1993) also reerred to the transcendent sel, describing successul individuals as transcenders who “move beyond the boundaries o their personal limitations by integrating  individual goals with larger ones, such as the welare o the amily, the community, humanity, the planet, or the cosmos” co smos” (p. 219) 219).. Similarly, Simil arly, Le and Levenson L evenson (2005) described sel-transcendence as “the ability to move beyond sel-centered consciousness, and to see things…  with a considerable measure mea sure o reedom rom  rom biological biologica l and social conditioning” (p. 444). Te recognition and ongoing awareness o a transcendent sel is a key  component o this capacity. Hamel et al. (2003) recently identied two components o Maslow’s (1971) metacognition (i.e., an expanded unitive consciousness). Te rst component is in-depth perception, described as the “ability to discern and explore the dierent aspects o one’s lie and lie in general, going beyond appearances” (Hamel et al., 2003, p. 11) and developing “a perception o realities that ordinary consciousness cannot perceive but that are common in contemplation” (p. 12). Te second component is holistic perception, dened as the “ability  to perceive one’s lie and lie in general rom a viewpoint independent o numerous attachments” (p. 11). It is urther urt her described as “an apprehension o realit realityy with all its contradictions and incompatibilities, a global integrative vision o the essential core o beings and events” (p. 12). Tese components describe cognitive abilities o  perception and awareness, the targets o which are oten said to exist outside o ordinary consciousness (to the extent that they are a re not perceived by the physical senses), including nonmaterialism, holism, interconnectedness, and transcendent aspects o the sel and others.

Collectively, these various abstractions represent what is currently described as transcendent. Conscious state expansion expa nsion (CSE) Te nal component o the current model is the ability to enter spiritual states o consciousness (e.g., pure consciousness, cosmic consciousness, oneness) at one’s own discretion. From a psychological perspective, the distinction between transcendental awareness and conscious state st ate expansion is i s well supported (art, 1975) 1975).. Te ormer must occur during the normal waking state,  while the latter involves the ability to transc transcend end this state and enter higher or spiritual states. A growing body  o research has demonstrated signicant dierences in brain unctioning between all levels and states o  consciousness, consciousn ess, including those associated with spiritual experiences and meditation (or a review, see Cahn & Polich, 2006; Vaitl et al., 2005). States commonly reerred to as spiritual include cosmic consciousness (Bucke, as cited in James, 1902/2002), pure consciousness (e.g., Gackenbach, 1992), 1992), and unitive consciousness c onsciousness (Maslow, (M aslow, 1964). Expanded or altere a ltered d states o consciousness consciousness are deeply rooted rooted aspects o religion and spirituality (James, 1902/2002; Maslow, 1964). As such, it is necessary to consider a potential mental ability that might underlie the experience o these states, as Emmons (2000a) correctly did in his own model o spiritual intelligence. Due to their phenomenological quality and potential spontaneous occurrence (James, 1902/2002; Maslow, 1964; Vaitl et al., 2005), however, the mere experience o such states does not constitute mental ability.  What is ar less debatable is the capacit capacityy or entering these states at one’s own discretion. Vaitl et al. (2005) identied a set o altered states o consciousness  which are triggere triggered d by one’ one’ss own mental exercises, as in meditation and relaxation. Over a century earlier,  James (1 (1902/2002) 902/2002) made a simila similarr observat observation, ion, noting that “the oncoming o mystical states may be acilitated by preliminary voluntary operations” (p. 415). He also described a “methodical cultivation” (p. 436) o such altered states in various religions, some o   which involve an a n “intellec “intellectual tual concentration” (p. 436). 436 ). Maslow (1964) agreed, stating that peak experiences are “to some extent under our own control” (p. 32). More recent psychological literature has reported individuals  who are highly skil skilled led at entering spiritual or mystica mysticall states o consciousness (e.g., Holmes, Solomon, Cappo, & Greenberg, 1983; Lehmann et al., 2001). Although Gardner (2000) contended that the ability to enter such

Spiritual Intelligence

International Journal of ranspersonal Studies  71

states is reective o h is bodily-kinesthetic intelligence, intelligence, a  cognitive component is no less evident and undamental to this capacity (James, 1902/2002; Vaitl et al., 2005).  Adaptive  Adapti ve applicat ions Myriad studies have demonstrated an adaptive role o spirituality in individuals suering rom a variety  o health conditions, including cancer (e.g., Holland et al., 1999), spinal cord injuries (e.g., Matheis et al., 2006), and substance abuse disorders (e.g., Piedmont, 2004), among others. Positive relationships have also been observed between spirituality and adaptability  to parental loss (Gree & Human, 2004) and career transitions (Duy & Blustein, 2005). It is rom these studies that the adaptive role o spiritual intelligence can be generally inerred. Tere are particular situatio situations ns in which spiritual intelligence would prove highly adaptive: the existential neurosis and the existential crisis. An existential neurosis is “characterized by the belie that one’s lie is meaningless” (Maddi, 1967, p. 313). Similarly, an existential vacuum can result rom a loss o meaning in lie or “the rustration o the will wi ll to meaning” meani ng” (Frankl, 1969, 1969, p. 45). Such existential crises can result rom isolation, the inevitability o death (Yalom, 1980), trauma (e.g., Goddard, 2004), rapid cultural change (e.g., Madison, 2006), and job loss (Hilpert, 1987). Highly developed levels o critical existential thinking and personal meaning production would prove particularly valuable, acilitating both contemplation o the crisis and creation o meaning within its ramework, potentially preventing  many crises rom ever ully developing. Maddi (1967) suggested that one possible source o an existential neurosis is a premorbid identity, in which “the person [considers] himsel to be nothing more than…an embodiment o biological needs” (p. 315), a conviction  which would wou ld be inhibited (i not entirely prevented) by  the perception o a transcendent sel.  Additional resea research rch has sugge suggested sted that the ability to construct meaning is adaptive in a variety o  circumstances, including depression, neuroticism, and suicidal ideation (e.g., Mascaro & Rosen, 2005). When aced with a stressor, personal meaning production acts as a coping method by allowing an individual to construct meaning and purpose within the stressul situation, thereby transorming it and reducing its negative impact. It has also been demonstrated that perceptions o interconnectedness and a transcendent sel are highly adaptive in substance abuse recovery  (Piedmont, 2004), depression (Ellermann & Reed,

2001), and coping with HIV/AIDS (e.g., Mellors, 1999). Although research is limited, transcendental awareness likely invokes a sense o security, acting as a source o coping when material resources are lacking  or insufcient. In regards to conscious state expansion, a variety o adaptive applications can be inerred rom research on meditation, which has revealed correlations  with reduced physiological indicators o stress (e.g.,  Alexander  Alex ander et e t al., a l., 1989). 1989). One can c an urther  urther postul postulate ate that th at this capacity would serve as a valuable coping method  when aced with a crisis (existential (existentia l or otherwise) due to its anxiety-inhibiting eects (Vaitl et al., 2005). Liespan developm development  ent  Accumulating evidence suggests that spiritual capacities can emerge in childhood. Piechowski (2001) noted many common themes in the spiritual experiences o children, including states o oneness and a sense o sel beyond physical reality. Hay and Nye (1998) identied children aged six to ten years  who could describe their own techniques or entering  higher states o consciousness, including silent gazing, prayer, and contemplating one’s origins and the origins o the world. Gackenbach (1992) maintained that the whole range o higher states o consciousness can be experienced by children, in some cases at an adult level, indicating the existence o consciousn consciousness ess savants. Evans and Wellman (2006) also discussed a childhood potential or existential reasoning which is contingent on the development o theory o mind.  Adolescence and young adulthood appear to be marked by an expansion o all spiritual capacities, resulting in part rom identity ormation and the emergence o abstract reasoning (Ellsworth, 1999; Fitzgerald, 2005; Helminiak, 1987). According to Parks (1986, 2000), individuals between the ages o  17 and 30 develop in their meaning-making abilities and become more aware o their conception o reality. Cook and Oltjenbruns (1982) also observed evidence o signicant development in existential skills during  high school. Tis development continues throughout adulthood, as abilities deepen and contribute to the awareness o a transcendent sel (Hamel et al., 2003; Helminiak, 1987). In later adulthood, abilities peak in many individuals as death approaches. According to ornstam (2005), later stages o lie are characterized by a redenition o the perception o time, space, lie, and death; the transcendence o the ego, o the physical body, and o material interests; and an increase in time spent meditating on lie.

International Journal of ranspersonal Studies 

King & DeCicco

72

Tis is not to say, however, that abilities cannot peak in earlier stages o development. As suggested by  Noble (2000) and Vaughan (2002), (2002), spiritual experiences contribute to the individual development o spiritual intelligence. Evidence, although limited, also supports the existence o individuals who are highly skilled and/ or gited in their spiritua l abilities (e.g., Emmons, 2000a; Gackenbach, 1992; Holmes et al., 1993; Lehmann et al., 2001; Lovecky, 1998; Piechowski, 1998), suggesting  that spiritual intelligence increases with both age and experience. Tus ar, spiritual intelligence has satised the three primary primar y criteria or or intelligence: a set o charac characteristic teristic mental abilities that are distinct distinc t rom preerred behaviors, the acilitation o adaptation and problem-solving, and development over the liespan (Gardner, 1983; Mayer et al., 2000; Sternberg, 1997). Empirical evidence urther suggests that spiritual experiences and their related sensations display physiological correlates in the brain, primarily in (but not limited to) the temporal lobes and limbic system (or reviews and more detailed summaries o related ndings, see d’Aquili & Newberg, 1999; Persinger, 1983, 1983, 2001; 2001; Ramachand R amachandran, ran, 1998). 1998). It has ha s also al so been discovered that altered brain activity occurs during  heightened states o consciousness (e.g., Cahn & Polich, 2006; Persinger, 1983; art, 1975; or a review, see Vaitl et al., 2005). Although urther research is required on spiritual capacities in particular, these ndings provide preliminary support or potential biological oundations o spiritual intelligence. As Emmons (2000a) noted, there is also a air amount o evidence or the evolutionary plausibility o spiritual intelligence,  with religious doctrine and dogma developing out o a  need to conceptualize, articulate, and interpret related spiritual experiences (Love, 2002). Te current model is not simply a reormulation or redenition o spirituality. Rather, its primary  purpose is the identication o those mental capacities and abilities related to human spirituality. o date, two sel-report measures o spiritual intelligence have been proposed (Amram & Dryer, 2007; Nasel, 2004), yet neither has ully complied with leading criteria  or intelligence (Gardner, 1983; Mayer et al., 2000; Sternberg, 1997). While it has been suggested that measures o spirituality also tap aspects o spiritual intelligence (Halama & Strizenec, 2004), Gardner’s (1983) criterion o psychometric evidence appears to be lacking. lackin g. Te subsequent studies were carried out in order to address these issues.

Spiritual Intelligence

Study 1: Scale Development  s suggested by Clark and Watson (1995), the development o a sound theoretical model on  which test te st items are based is a crucial rst step in scale sc ale development. Te vast majority o researchers rely on a  thorough literature review o the subject in order to iner rom previous research a model o the construct. Tis has been the current approach. Te purpose o Study 1  was to develop and test a preliminar preliminaryy sel-report sel-report measure o spiritual intelligence. Method  Item generation. An original origina l pool o 84 items was developed based on the current theoretical conception c onception o  spiritual intelligence. Many Ma ny items were were modelled based on previously established measures o spirituality, meaning, and transcendence, while others were generated in order to complete the ull ranges o the our capacities. Te initial item pool was over-inclusive so as to avoid the exclusion o potential indicators o the construct (Clark  & Watson, 1995). A multiple-choice, Likert-type scale  was utilized, with responses ranging on a scale o 0 to 4, representing the extent to which each statement was true or the respondent. In total, 12 reverse-coded items  were included as indicators o internal reliability. Te initial item pool was reviewed by 34 adults (including  proessors o psychology, students, and members o the community) or logic and readability. Items were edited based on eedback, resulting in a preliminary dra t o the Spiritual Intelligence Sel-Report Inventory (SISRI). Participants. Respondents were 488 emales and 131 males (N = 619) enrolled in undergraduate psychology courses c ourses at rent rent University in Peterborough, Ontario, Canada. Te mean age was 22.51 years (SD = 5.51; range = 17 to 59). Measures.. Participants were asked to provide Measures demographic inormation (including sex and age) beore completing the preliminary 84-item Spiritual Intelligence Sel-Report Inventory (with 21 items measuring each o  the our components o spiritual intelligence). i ntelligence). Procedure. Participation took place in classroom settings and lasted approximately 25 minutes. Each participant was asked to read and sign a consent orm prior to participation ensuring condentiality and the right to withdraw at any time without penalty penalt y. Results Properties of the original 84-item SISRI. Descriptive statistics and response distributions were rst examined or all 84 items. Although none reached signicance, slight skewness was observed or 15 o 

 A 

International Journal of ranspersonal Studies  73

the items, which were deemed good candidates or removal. Cronbach’s alpha was .97, suggesting very high internal consistency and reliability (a minimum o .80 is recommended; Clark & Watson, 1995; Worthington & Whittaker, 2006). Te average inter-item correlation  was .30, which alls a lls in i n the suggested range o .15 .15 to .50 (Clark & Watson, 1995). Exploratory factor analysis.  All responses or the 84-item pool were subjected to a principal components analysis (EFA) with Varimax normalized rotation. Six actors were initially extracted and actor loadings o .35 or higher were deemed signicant (Clark  & Watson, 1995). All eigenvalues met the suggested minimum value o 1.0 in order or their corresponding  actors to be considered or retention (Clark & Watson, 1995; Worthington & Whittaker, 2006). Te third actor extracted was composed o all 12 reverse-coded items, suggesting that these items were unstable. One o these items displayed a cross-loading on Factor 4 and  was considered or retention. Te three items loading on the sixth actor had no theoretical connection, leading  to this actor’s removal. Items loading on actors 1, 2, 4, and 5 corresponded primarily to critical existential thinking, conscious state expansion, personal meaning  production, and transcendental awareness, respectively. Item Retention. Te primary criterion or item retention was theoretical necessity (as suggested by  Clark & Watson, 1995). As such, items were removed only i such removal did not result in inadequate model representation. All items with converging cross-loadings (with dierences o less than .15; Clark & Watson, 1995) were considered or removal, as were those which loaded on theoretically unrelated actors or displayed no signicant loadings whatsoever. Finally, a number o  items were removed in order to reduce redundancy and to obtain a desirable scale length. In total, 42 items were retained, including 12 items items measuring critical existentia l thinking, 11 items measuring conscious state expansion, 9 items measuring personal meaning mea ning production, and 10 items measuring transcendental awareness. Properties of the Reduced 42-Item SISRI.  Alpha or the 42-item pool was .96, which was slightly  lower than in the original 84-item pool but considered an improvement to the data (Clark & Watson, 1995). Te average inter-item correlation was .36. When subjected to a second principal components EFA, all our actors displayed eigenvalues above 1.0 and no residual correlations exceeded .14, adding condence to the actors and items retained (Clark & Watson, 1995). 1995).

Study 2: Scale Validity  he primary purpose o Study 2 was to investigate the actor structure o the Spiritual Intelligence Sel-Report Inventory (SISRI) in a subsequent sample. Construct validity val idity was examined by including additional measures o theoretically related and unrelated constructs (DeVellis, 1991). As is standard in scale development, social desirability desirability was also examined. exa mined. Hypothesis 1. Based on the theoretical conception o spiritual intelligence and the actor structure observed in Study 1, it was hypothesized that a our-actor model o spiritual intelligence would reveal adequate t to the data. Hypothesis 2. Based on the current denition o personal meaning production, it  was hypothesized hypothesi zed that t hat a signicant signi cant positive relationship relat ionship  would be observed between this subscale o the SISRI and a measure o personal meaning. mea ning. Hypothesis 3. Given that metapersonal sel-construal has been described as a more spiritual orm o sel-reerence (DeCicco & Stroink, 2007), it was hypothesized that a signicant positive relationship would be observed between the SISRI and the metapersonal sel. Hypothesis 4. Based on the current denition o conscious state expansion, it  was hypothesized hypothesi zed that t hat a signicant signi cant positive relationship relat ionship  would be observed between mystical experiences and this subscale o the SISRI. Hypothesis 5. Te SISRI was expected to display a non-signicant to low correlation  with extrinsic religiosity and a positive yet low to moderate correlation with intrinsic religiosity. Tese predictions were based on denitions o religiosity which tend to relate the intrinsic orientation more closely to spirituality (e.g., Pargament, 1997). 1997). Method  Participants. Respondents were 231 emales and 74 males (N = 305) enrolled in undergraduate psychology courses c ourses at rent University in Peterborough, Ontario, Canada. Te mean age was 25.56 years (SD = 10.93; range = 18 to 63). Demographics. A one-page survey o basic demographic inormation accounted or sex, age, ethnicity, relationship status, and level o education. Measures. A total o six measures were utilized to evaluate participants.

International Journal of ranspersonal Studies 

King & DeCicco

74



Te Spiritual Intelligence Self-Report Inventory  (SISRI). Te 42-item drat o the SISRI, as a s developed in Study 1, was utilized in Study 2. Te Meaning in Life Questionnaire (MLQ;  Steger, Frazier, Oishi, & Kaler, 2006). Te MLQ  is a 10-item sel-report measure o personal meaning 

composed o two subscales: presence o meaning (a = .88) and search or meaning ( a = .90). Validity and reliability have been supported by Steger et al. (2006).

Te Metapersonal Self Scale (MPS; DeCicco & Stroink, 2007). Te MPS is a 10-item (a = .89) selreport measure o metapersonal sel-construal, which is the interpretation o one’s sel as connected to all lie. Tis scale was ound to be high in convergent and discriminant validity by DeCicco and Stroink (2007). Te Mysticism Scale – Research Form D (MSD;  Hood, 1975). Te MSD is a 32-item (a = .94) selreport measure o mystical and spiritual experiences, particularly those involving oneness, peace, timelessness, and wonder. While Hood (1975) suggested two actors o intense experience o unity and aectively charged religious revelation, Hood, Morris, and Watson (1993) later proposed three actors o extrovertive mysticism, religious interpretation, and introvertive mysticism. Scale validity and reliability have been well-supported by  Hood (1975) and Hood et al. (1993). Te Age Universal Intrinsic-Extrinsic Intrinsic-Extrinsic Religiosity  Scale (AUIE; Gorsuch & Venable, 1983). Te AUIE is a 19-item, two-actor sel-report measure o intrinsic (a = .89) and extrinsic (a = .77) religious orientations. Validity and reliability were supported by Gorsuch and Venable (1983). Te Balanced Inventory of Desirable Responding  (BIDR; Paulhus, 1984). Te BIDR is a 40-item selreport measure composed o two actors related to socially desirable responding: sel-deception (a = .72) and impression management (a = .72). Internal and external validity o the BIDR have been supported by  Paulhus (1984) (1984) and Lanyon L anyon and Carle Ca rle (2007). Procedure. Participation took place in classroom settings and lasted approximately 45 minutes. Each participant was asked to read and sign a consent

orm prior to participation ensuring condentiality and the right to withdraw at any time without penalty. Te ordering o questionnaires varied across participants so as to reduce potential eects on response patterns (e.g., priming). Results Properties of the 42-Item SISRI. Descriptive statistics and response distributions were rst examined or the 42-item pool. No items displayed signicant skewness or kurtosis. Alpha or the SISRI-42 was .96,  which reects observations in Study 1. Te average interinteritem correlation was .36. Confrmatory actor analysis. Te structural equation modelling (SEM) module o Statistica 7.0 (Statsot, 2006) was utilized or the conrmatory  actor analysis (CFA). Te our-actor model observed in Study 1 was investigated or its t to the data in Study 2. Parameter estimates (maximum likelihood) or all 42 maniest variables were signicant. Te discrepancy unction  unction or the our-actor model was 6.96. In addition to the chi-square goodness-o-t (X ²), the ollowing t indices were examined (see able 1): the root mean square error o approximation (RMSEA), the goodness-o-t goodness- o-t index (GFI), (GFI), the adjusted goodnesso-t index (AGFI), (AGFI), the standardized standardiz ed root mean square squa re residual (SRMSR), and the c omparative t index (CFI). Tese t indices are commonly employed measures o  CFA model t (Schermelleh-Engel & Moosbrugger, 2003; abachnick abachnick & Fidell, 2007). Te ollowing cuto values were employed to determine adequacy: a  maximum X ²/d ratio o 2.0; a maximum SRMSR o  .08; a minimum CFI o .95; a maximum RMSEA o  .10; an RMSEA lower condence limit o close to .05; a minimum AGFI o .85; and a minimum GFI o .90 (Schermelleh-Engel & Moosbrugger, 2003; abachnick  & Fidell, 2007).

able 1

Fit Indices or Confrmatory Factor Analysis o the 42-Item SISRI 

_________________________ ____________ _________________________ _________________________ __________________________ __________________________ _______________ __ X ²

RMSEA LCL UCL UCL SRMSR GFI AGFI CFI d   _________________________ ____________ _________________________ _________________________ __________________________ __________________________ _______________ __ Value 2108.72* .080 .077 .084 .067 .726 .695 .832 813 _________________________ ____________ _________________________ _________________________ __________________________ __________________________ _______________ __ Note. LCL = RMSEA Lower Condence Limit; UCL = R MSEA Upper Condence Limit. * p < .0001

Spiritual Intelligence

International Journal of ranspersonal Studies  75

abachnick and Fidell (2007) noted that with large sample sizes (over 200), the chi-square value is almost always signicant, signi cant, and is thereore a poor indicator indicator o model t. Te chi-square/d ratio was calculated in order to gain a more meaningul summary o these statistics. statistic s. Te our-actor model displayed a ratio o 2.59, 2.59,  which exceeded the recommended maxi maximum mum value o  2.0 or good t (abachnick & Fidell, 2007). While the SRMSR and RMSEA met cut-os or adequate t, the GFI, AGFI, and CFI did not, suggesting poor model t (see able 1). Scale modifcation. Based on high residual correlations, high item correlations, and redundancy, ve items were removed rom the CE subscale; our items were removed rom the PMP subscale; three items  were removed rom t he A subscale; and six items were removed rom the CSE subscale. Tis let a total o  24 items or the nal drat o the SISRI (subsequently  dubbed the SISRI-24; see Appendix). Properties o the SISRI-24. Te nal 24-item pool displayed an alpha o .92, which represents a more appropriate level o internal reliability (Clark & Wa Watson, tson, 1995) 995).. Individual Individua l subscales subsca les o CE CE,, PMP, PMP, A, and a nd CSE also displayed d isplayed adequate alpha coefcients co efcients o .78, .78, .87, .87, and .91, respectively. Te average inter-item correlation  was .34, with split-hal reliability at the .9 .91 1 level. Tese analyses suggest excellent psychometric properties o the SISRI-24.  A conrmatory actor analysis o the SISRI24 revealed signicant parameter estimates or all 24 variables (see Figure 1) with a discrepancy unction o 1.53, indicating better model t or the SISRI-24 compared to the 42-item version (abachnick & Fidell, 2007). Te X ²/d ratio was 1.89, which ell under the recommended maximum value o 2.0 or good model t (abachnick & Fidell, 2007). Te SRMSR, RMSEA,

and AGFI also met their recommended cut-o values (see able 2), urther supporting good model t. Given that the GFI and CFI closely approached their cut-o  values, it can be concluded that the our-actor model displayed adequate t to the data. In order to ensure adequate item loadings, the nal 24-item pool was also subjected to a principal components analysis. Four actors were extracted with eigenvalues supporting retention o all our actors. All actor loadings were signicant above the .50 level (see able 3). Although six items cross-loaded, they diered rom their highest loadings by at least .13, with all o  these loadings in the .35 to .40 range. Tese ndings add urther condence to the actor structure and item retention o the SISRI-24. All subsequent analyses were thereore based on participant responses or the 24-item pool. Scale validity.  All bivariate correlations are presented in able 4. In relation to the MLQ search or meaning subscale, low to moderate correlations with the total spiritual intelligence score (SI) and subscale scores on the SISRI-24 were observed. In particular, CE  was the most signica signicantly ntly related subscale, r = .39 (p < .001). Correlations with the MLQ presence o meaning  subscale were ar more signicant overall. SI correlated at r = .44 (p < .001), .001), ollowed closely by A and CSE. CSE . CE did not correlate signicantly with presence o meaning. In complete contrast, PMP correlated positively and signicantly signica ntly with presence o mea ning, r = .65 (p < .01), .01), yet displayed no signicant correlation with search or meaning. Metapersonal sel-construal displayed moderate to high positive correlations with SI and all subscales. O the subscales, the highest correlation occurred with A, r = .63 (p < .01). otal mystical experiences, including all potential MSD subscales,

able 2

Fit Indices or Confrmatory Factor Analysis o the SISRI-24 

_________________________ _____________ _________________________ __________________________ __________________________ __________________________ ______________ _ X ²

RMSEA LCL UCL UCL SRMSR GFI AGFI CFI d   _________________________ _____________ _________________________ __________________________ __________________________ __________________________ ______________ _ Value 464.68* .055 .047 .062 .056 .886 .861 .934 246 _________________________ _____________ _________________________ __________________________ __________________________ __________________________ ______________ _ Note. LCL = RMSEA Lower Condence Limit; UCL = RMSEA Upper Condence Limit. * p < .0001

76

International Journal of ranspersonal Studies 

King & DeCicco

Critical Existential Tinking 

R.E.

1

.75

3

.70

5

.72

_______________________________________

9

.65

13 .68* 17 .50* 21

.52

Item

R.E.

7

.42

11

.66

15

.71

19

.50

23

.62

Item

R.E.

2

.46

6

.68

10

.44

14

.48

18 .72* 20 .73* 22

.54

Item Factor 1 Factor 2 Factor 3 Factor 4 _______________________________________ 1 .65* .14 -.11 .14 2 .38 .14 .28 .59* 3 .73* .01 .07 .07 4 .18 .77* .21 .30 5 .52* .14 .39 .05 6 .07 .24 .04 .66* 7 .16 .22 .63* .36 8 .11 .85* .22 .21 9 .53* .22 .18 .20 10 .31 .16 .37 .59* 11 .06 .21 .78* -.03 12 .11 .83* .22 .15 13 .58* .15 .33 .25 14 .19 .30 .15 .66* 15 -.07 .05 .53* .40 16 .13 .81* .12 .17 17 .54* .29 .12 .31 18 .24 .08 .29 .63* 19 .21 .32 .68* .14 20 .29 .26 -.01 .70* 21 .65* -.02 -.03 -.03 .18 22 .21 .22 .20 .69* 23 .04 .12 .57* .39 24 .11 .75* .13 .25 Eigenvalues 8.86 2.12 1.63 1.33 _______________________________________ * All marked loadings > .50

Item

R.E.

4

.27

8

.17

12

.26

16

.39

24

.38

.50* .55* .53* .59* .69*

.54 .75

.60*

Personal Meaning  Making 

.77*

.76* .59* .54* .71* .62*

.53*

.74*

ranscendental .62*  Awareness  Awa reness

able 3

Item

.74* .57* .75* .72* .68*

.49 .47

Factor Loadings (Principal Components, Varimax Normalized) or the SISRI-24 

.61*

Conscious State Expansion

Figure 1.

.85* .91* .86* .78* .72*

CFA our-actor our-actor model or the SISRI-24 R.E. = Residual Error *Parameter *Para meter estimates signicant at p
View more...

Comments

Copyright ©2017 KUPDF Inc.
SUPPORT KUPDF