OET Reading Test 2 - Part B

January 10, 2018 | Author: LOTSOFTESTS | Category: Alzheimer's Disease, Dementia, Climate, Infection, Public Health
Share Embed Donate


Short Description

A lot more OET tests at lotsoftests.com...

Description

Occupational English Test

READING SUB-TEST Part B - Text Booklet Practice test You must record your answers for Part B on the multiple-choice answer sheet using 2B pencil. Please print in BLOCK LETTERS

Candidate number







Family name



Other name(s)



City



Date of test

Candidate’s signature



YOU MUST NOT REMOVE OET MATERIAL FROM THE TEST ROOM.

The OET Centre GPO Box 372 Melbourne VIC 3001 Australia © OET Centre — Practice test

Telephone: +61 3 9652 0800 Facsimile: +61 3 9654 5329 www.occupationalenglishtest.org

ABN 84 434 201 642

READING PART B Instructions TIME LIMIT: 45 MINUTES

There are TWO reading texts in Part B. After each of the texts you will find a number of questions or unfinished statements about the text, each with four suggested answers or ways of finishing. You must choose the ONE which you think fits best. For each question, 1-20, indicate on your answer sheet the letter A, B, C or D against the number of the question. Answer ALL questions. Marks are NOT deducted for incorrect answers. NOTE: You must complete your Answer Sheet for Part B within the 45 minutes allowed for this part of the sub-test.

NOW TURN TO THE NEXT PAGE FOR TEXTS AND QUESTIONS

Reading Part B

Part B : Multiple Choice Questions Time Limit: 20~25 Minutes   Alzheimer  Disease   Author:  Margaret  Gatz   Source:  Public  Library  of  Open  Science  

  Paragraph  1   Physicians  now  commonly  advise  older  adults  to  engage  in  mentally  stimulating   activity  as  a  way  of  reducing  their  risk  of  dementia.  Indeed,  the  recommendation   is  often  followed  by  the  acknowledgment  that  evidence  of  benefit  is  still  lacking,   but  “it  can’t  hurt.”  What  could  possibly  be  the  problem  with  older  adults   spending  their  time  doing  crossword  puzzles  and  anagrams,  completing  puzzles,   or  testing  their  reaction  time  on  a  computer?  In  certain  respects,  there  is  no   problem.  Patients  will  probably  improve  at  the  targeted  skills,  and  may  feel   good—particularly  if  the  activity  is  both  challenging  and  successfully  completed.     Paragraph  2   But  can  it  hurt?  Possibly.  There  are  two  ways  that  encouraging  mental  activity   programs  might  do  more  harm  than  good.  First,  they  can  falsely  raise   expectations.  Second,  individuals  who  do  develop  dementia  might  be  blamed  for   their  condition.  When  heavy  smokers  get  lung  cancer,  they  are  sometimes  seen   as  having  contributed  to  their  own  fates.  People  with  Alzheimer  disease  might   similarly  be  viewed  as  having  brought  it  on  themselves  through  failure  to   exercise  their  brains.     Paragraph  3   There  is  some  evidence  to  support  the  idea  that  mental  exercise  can  improve   one’s  chances  of  escaping  Alzheimer  disease.  Having  more  years  of  education  has   been  shown  to  be  related  to  a  lower  prevalence  of  Alzheimer  disease.  Typically,   the  risk  of  Alzheimer  disease  is  two  to  four  times  higher  in  those  who  have  fewer   years  of  education,  as  compared  to  those  who  have  more  years  of  education.   Other  epidemiological  studies,  although  with  less  consistency,  have  suggested   that  those  who  engage  in  more  leisure  activities  have  a  lower  prevalence  and   incidence  of  Alzheimer  disease.  Additionally,  longitudinal  studies  have  found   that  older  adults  without  dementia  who  participate  in  more  intellectually   challenging  daily  activities  show  less  decline  over  time  on  various  tests  of   cognitive  performance.     Paragraph  4   However,  both  education  and  leisure  activities  are  imperfect  measures  of  mental   exercise.  For  instance,  leisure  activities  represent  a  combination  of  influences.   Not  only  is  there  mental  activation,  but  there  may  also  be  broader  health  effects,   including  stress  reduction  and  improved  vascular  health—  both  of  which  may   contribute  to  reducing  dementia  risk.  It  could  also  be  that  a  third  factor,  such  as   intelligence,  leads  to  greater  levels  of  education  and  more  engagement  in   cognitively  stimulating  activities,  and  independently,  to  lower  risk  of  dementia.   Research  in  Scotland,  for  example,  showed  that  IQ  test  scores  at  age  11  were   predictive  of  future  dementia  risk  .     This resource was developed by OET Online and is subject to copyright © Website: http://oetonline.com.au Email: [email protected]

Reading Part B

Paragraph  5   The  concept  of  cognitive  reserve  is  often  used  to  explain  why  education  and   mental  stimulation  are  beneficial.  The  term  cognitive  reserve  is  sometimes  taken   to  refer  directly  to  brain  size  or  to  synaptic  density  in  the  cortex.  At  other  times,   cognitive  reserve  is  defined  as  the  ability  to  compensate  for  acquired  brain   pathology.  Taken  together,  the  evidence  is  very  suggestive  that  having  greater   cognitive  reserve  is  related  to  a  reduced  risk  of  Alzheimer  disease.  But  the   evidence  that  mental  exercise  can  increase  cognitive  reserve  and  keep  dementia   at  bay  is  weaker.  In  addition,  people  with  greater  cognitive  reserve  may  choose   mentally  stimulating  leisure  activities  and  jobs,  which  makes  is  difficult  to   precisely  determine  whether  mentally  stimulating  activities  alone  can  reduce   dementia  risk.       Paragraph  6   Cognitive  training  has  demonstrable  effects  on  performance,  on  views  of  self,  and   on  brain  function—but  the  results  are  very  specific  to  the  skills  that  are  trained,   and  it  is  as  yet  entirely  unknown  whether  there  is  any  effect  on  when   or  whether  an  individual  develops  Alzheimer  disease.  Further,  the  types  of  skills   taught  by  practicing  mental  puzzles  may  be  less  helpful  in  everyday  life  than   more  straightforward  techniques,  such  as  concentrating,  or  taking  notes,  or   putting  objects  in  the  same  place  each  time  so  that  they  won’t  be  lost.     Paragraph  7   So  far,  there  is  little  evidence  that  mental  practice  will  help  prevent  the   development  of  dementia.  There  is  better  evidence  that  good  brain  health  is   determined  by  multiple  factors,  that  brain  development  early  in  life  matters,   and  that  genetic  influences  are  of  great  importance  in  accounting  for  individual   differences  in  cognitive  reserve  and  in  explaining  who  develops  Alzheimer   disease  and  who  does  not.  At  least  half  of  the  explanation  for  individual   differences  in  susceptibility  to  Alzheimer  disease  is  genetic,  although  the  genes   involved  have  not  yet  been  completely  discovered.  The  balance  of  the   explanation  lies  in  environmental  influences  and  behavioral  health  practices,   alone  or  in  interaction  with  genetic  factors.  However,  at  this  stage,  there  is  no   convincing  evidence  that  memory  practice  and  other  cognitively  stimulating   activities  are  sufficient  to  prevent  Alzheimer  disease;  it  is  not  just  a  case  of  “use  it   or  lose  it.”    

This resource was developed by OET Online and is subject to copyright © Website: http://oetonline.com.au Email: [email protected]

Reading Part B

Part B : Multiple Choice Questions 1. According to paragraph 1, which of the following statements matches the opinion of most doctors? a. Mentally stimulating activities are of little use b. The risk of dementia can be reduced by doing mentally stimulating activities c. The benefits of mentally stimulating activities are not yet proven d. Mentally stimulating activities do more harm than good 2. In paragraph 2, the author expresses the opinion that ……. a. Mentally stimulating activities may offer false hope b. Dementia sufferers often blame themselves for their condition c. Alzheimer’s disease may be caused lack of mental exercise d. Mentally stimulating activities do more harm than good 3. In paragraph 3, which of the following does not match the information on research into Alzheimer disease? a. People with less education have a higher risk of Alzheimer disease b. Cognitive performance can be enhanced by regularly doing activities which are mentally challenging c. Having more education reduces the risk of Alzheimer disease d. Regular involvement in leisure activities may reduce the risk of Alzheimer disease 4. According to paragraph 4, which of the following statements is false? a. The impact of education and leisure is difficult to measure b. Better vascular health and reduced stress can decrease the risk of dementia c. People with higher IQ scores may be less likely to suffer from dementia d. Cognitively stimulating activities reduce dementia risk 5. Which of the following is closest in meaning to the expression: keep dementia at bay? a. delay the onset of dementia b. cure dementia c. reduce the severity of dementia d. treat dementia 6. Which of the following phrases best summarises the main idea presented in paragraph 6? a. The effect cognitive training has on Alzheimer disease is limited b. Doing mental puzzles may not be as beneficial as concentrating in everyday life c. Cognitive training improves brain performance d. The effect cognitive training has on Alzheimer disease is indefinite

This resource was developed by OET Online and is subject to copyright © Website: http://oetonline.com.au Email: [email protected]

Reading Part B

7. According to paragraph 7, which of the following is correct regarding the development of dementia? a. Genetic factors are the most significant b. Environmental factors interact with behavioural factors in determining susceptibility to Alzheimer disease c. Good brain health can reduce the risk of developing Alzheimer disease d. None of the above 8. Which of the following would be the best alternative title for the essay? a. New developments in Alzheimer research b. Benefits of education in fighting Alzheimer disease c. Doubts regarding mental exercise as a preventive measure for Alzheimer disease d. The importance of cognitive training in preventing early onset of Alzheimer disease

This resource was developed by OET Online and is subject to copyright © Website: http://oetonline.com.au Email: [email protected]

Reading Part B

Part  B  :  Multiple  Choice  Questions      

 

Time  Limit:  20~25  Minutes  

Infectious  Diseases  and  Climatic  Influences  

 

Authors:  Bernard  Cazelles,  Simon  Hales   Source:  Public  Library  of  Open  Science  

  Paragraph  1   Complex  dynamic  relationships  between  humans,  pathogens,  and  the   environment  lead  to  the  emergence  of  new  diseases  and  the  re-­‐emergence  of  old   ones.  Due  to  concern  about  the  impact  of  increasing  global  climate  variability   and  change,  many  recent  studies  have  focused  on  relationships  between   infectious  disease  and  climate.       Paragraph  2   Climate  can  be  an  important  determinant  of  vector-­‐borne  disease  epidemics:   geographic  and  seasonal  patterns  of  infectious  disease  incidence  are  often,   though  not  always,  driven  by  climate  factors.  Mosquito-­‐  borne  diseases,  such  as   malaria,  dengue  fever,  and  Ross  River  virus,  typically  show  strong  seasonal  and   geographic  patterns,  as  do  some  intestine  diseases.  These  patterns  are   unsurprising,  given  the  influence  of  climate  on  pathogen  replication,  vector  and   disease  reservoir  populations,  and  human  societies.  In  Sweden,  a  trend  toward   milder  winters  and  early  spring  arrival  may  be  implicated  in  an  increased   incidence  of  tick-­‐borne  encephalitis.  The  recent  resurgence  of  malaria  in  the  East   African  highlands  may  be  explained  by  increasing  temperatures  in  that  region.   However,  as  yet  there  are  relatively  few  studies  showing  clear  climatic   influences  on  infectious  diseases  at  interannual  or  longer  timescales.     Paragraph  3   The  semi-­‐regular  El  Niño  climate  cycle,  centred  on  the  Pacific  Ocean,  has  an   important  influence  on  interannual  climate  patterns  in  many  parts  of  the  world.   This  makes  El  Niño  an  attractive,  albeit  imperfect,  analogue  for  the  effects  of   global  climate  change.  In  Peru,  daily  admissions  for  diarrhoea  increased  by  more   than  2-­‐fold  during  an  El  Niño  event,  compared  with  expected  trends  based  on  the   previous  five  years.  There  is  evidence  of  a  relationship  between  El  Niño  and  the   timing  of  cholera  epidemics  in  Peru  and  Bangladesh;  of  ciguatera  in  the  Pacific   islands;  of  Ross  River  virus  epidemics  in  Australia;  and  of  dengue  and  malaria   epidemics  in  several  countries.  The  onset  of  meningococcal  meningitis  in  Mali  is   associated  with  large-­‐scale  atmospheric  circulation.     Paragraph  4   These  studies  were  performed  mostly  at  country  scale,  reflecting  the  availability   of  data  sources  and,  perhaps,  the  geographically  local  effects  of  El  Niño  on   climate.  In  part  because  of  this  geographic  “patchiness”  of  the  epidemiological   evidence,  the  identification  of  climatic  factors  in  infectious  disease  dynamics,  and   the  relative  importance  of  the  different  factors,  remains  controversial.  For   example,  it  has  been  suggested  that  climate  trends  are  unlikely  to  contribute  to   the  timing  of  dengue  epidemics  in  Thailand.  However,  recent  work  has  shown  a   strong  but  transient  association  between  dengue  incidence  and  El  Niño  in   This resource was developed by OET Online and is subject to copyright © Website: http://oetonline.com.au Email: [email protected]

Reading Part B

Thailand.  This  association  may  possibly  be  caused  by  a  “pacemaker-­‐like”  effect   in  which  intrinsic  disease  dynamics  interact  with  climate  variations  driven  by  El   Niño  to  propagate  travelling  waves  of  infection.           Paragraph  5   A  new  study  on  cutaneous  leishmaniasis  by  Chaves  and  Pascual  also  provides   fresh  evidence  of  a  relationship  between  climate  and  vector-­‐borne  disease.   Chaves  and  Pascual  use  a  range  of  mathematical  tools  to  illustrate  a  clear   relationship  between  climatic  variables  and  the  dynamics  of  cutaneous   leishmaniasis,  a  skin  infection  transmitted  by  sandflies.  In  Costa  Rica,  cutaneous   leishmaniasis  displays  three-­‐year  cycles  that  coincide  with  those  of  El  Niño.   Chaves  and  Pascual  use  this  newly  demonstrated  association  to  enhance  the   forecasting  ability  of  their  models  and  to  predict  the  epidemics  of  leishmaniasis   up  to  one  year  ahead.  Interestingly,  El  Niño  was  a  better  predictor  of  disease   than  temperature,  possibly  because  this  large-­‐scale  index  integrates  numerous   environmental  processes  and  so  is  a  more  biologically  relevant  measure  than   local  temperature.  As  the  authors  note,  the  link  between  El  Niño  and  epidemics   of  leishmaniasis  might  be  explained  by  large-­‐scale  climate  effects  on  population   susceptibility.  Susceptibility,  in  turn,  may  be  related  to  lack  of  specific  immunity   or  poor  nutritional  status,  both  of  which  are  plausibly  influenced  by  climate.     Paragraph  6   Chaves  and  Pascual  have  identified  a  robust  relationship  between  climate  and   disease,  with  changes  over  time  in  average  incidence  and  in  cyclic  components.   The  dynamics  of  cutaneous  leishmaniasis  evolve  coherently  with  climatic   variables  including  temperature  and  El  Niño  indices,  demonstrating  a  strong   association  between  these  variables,  particularly  after  1996.  Long-­‐term  changes   in  climate,  human  demography,  and  social  features  of  human  populations  have   large  effects  on  the  dynamics  of  epidemics  as  underlined  by  the  analyses  of  some   large  datasets  on  whooping  cough  and  measles.  Another  illuminating  example  is   the  transient  relationship  between  cholera  prevalence  and  El  Niño  oscillations.   In  Bangladesh,  early  in  the  20th  century,  cholera  and  El  Niño  appeared   unrelated,  yet  a  strong  association  emerged  in  1980–2001.  Transient   relationships  between  climate  and  infectious  disease  may  be  caused  by   interactions  between  climate  and  intrinsic  disease  mechanisms  such  as   temporary  immunity.  If  population  susceptibility  is  low,  even  large  increases  in   transmission  potential  due  to  climate  forcing  will  not  result  in  a  large  epidemic.     Paragraph  7   A  deeper  understanding  of  infectious  disease  dynamics  is  important  in  order  to   forecast,  and  perhaps  forestall,  the  effects  of  dramatic  global  social  and   environmental  changes.  Conventional  statistical  methods  may  fail  to  reveal   a  relationship  between  climate  and  health  when  discontinuous  associations  are   present.  Because  classical  methods  quantify  average  associations  over  the  entire   dataset,  they  may  not  be  adequate  to  decipher  long-­‐term  but  discontinuous   relationships  between  environmental  exposures  and  human  health.  On  the  other   hand,  relationships  between  climate  and  disease  could  signal  problems  for   This resource was developed by OET Online and is subject to copyright © Website: http://oetonline.com.au Email: [email protected]

Reading Part B

disease  prediction.  Unless  all  important  effects  are  accounted  for,  dynamic   forecast  models  may  prove  to  have  a  limited  shelf  life.  !     Part B : Multiple Choice Questions 1. According to paragraph 2, which of the following is true? a. The incidence of infectious diseases is rarely caused by climatic factors. b. Seasonal variations and geography always lead to increases in mosquito borne diseases. c. An increase in the rate of tick-borne encephalitis has been caused by milder winters and early arrival spring in Sweden. d. Malaria may have reappeared in East African highlands due to higher temperatures. 2. Which of the following would be the most appropriate heading for the paragraph 2? a. The link between global warming and disease epidemics . b. The strong relationship between climate and outbreaks of disease. c. The unexpected influence of climate on infectious diseases. d. The need for further research into climate change and infectious diseases. 3. Which of the following is closest in meaning to the expression relatively few? a. comparatively few b. several c. quite a few d. three 4. In paragraph 3, which of the following is not true? a. In Peru, the El Nino event led to increased rates of diarrhoea . b. El-Nino has a significant yearly effect on global climate patterns. c. Outbreaks of cholera in Bangladesh and Peru can be linked to El Nino. d. Meningococcal meningitis in Mali is influenced by weather patterns. 5. The main point the author wishes to raise in paragraph 4 is…………. a. Despite differing opinions, there is strong current evidence linking climate factors and infectious disease. b. There is insufficient data to determine how significant climatic factors are on infectious disease. c. The link between climate trends and disease epidemics is still inconclusive. d. There is no connection between climatic trends and dengue fever in Thailand.

This resource was developed by OET Online and is subject to copyright © Website: http://oetonline.com.au Email: [email protected]

Reading Part B

6. According to paragraph 5 which of the following statements is correct? a. Outbreaks of cutaneous leishmaniasis in Costa Rica correspond with El Nino events. b. The mathematical tools used by Chaves and Pascual demonstrate the link between sandflies and cutaneous leishmaniasis. c. Research by Chaves and Pascual will allow for annual prediction of leishmaniasis outbreaks. d. El Nino is an accurate predictor disease due its complexity and biological relevance. 7. Which of the following is closest in meaning to the word plausibly? a. definitely b. possibly c. regularly d. occasionally 8. According to paragraph 6, which of the following statements is correct? a. The relationship between climate and disease is constant. b. Outbreaks of cholera appear to be unrelated to El Nino patterns. c. The dynamics of epidemics are affected by changes in population, society and weather. d. Large epidemics rarely occur due to climate changes. 9. Which of the following is closest in meaning to transient relationship? a. Strong relationship b. Long term relationship c. Close relationship d. Non-permanent relationship 10. In paragraph 7, which of the following statements is most correct? a. There may be weaknesses in orthodox statistical methods. b. The dynamics of infectious diseases in well understood. c. The data measuring long term relationships between human health and environmental exposure is inadequate. d. Relationships between climate and disease will make disease prediction difficult.

This resource was developed by OET Online and is subject to copyright © Website: http://oetonline.com.au Email: [email protected]

Reading Part B

Answer  Key     1.  c  2.  a  3.  b  4.  d  5.  a  6.  d  7.a  8.  c   Question  1   a) Incorrect:  No,  they  believe  it  may  help   b) Incorrect:    This  is  their  advice,  but  they  acknowledge  there  is  no  evidence     c) Correct:  See  highlight   d) Incorrect:  No,  (this  is  the  author’s  opinion  in  paragraph  2)   Question  2   a) Correct:  synonym:  (may  offer  false  hope=  can  falsely  raise  expectations)   b) Incorrect:  Not  mentioned   c) Incorrect:  No,  some  people  may  believe  this,  but  not  the  author   d) Incorrect:  No,  they  might  i.e  it  is  possible   Question  3   a) Incorrect:  Matches   b) Correct:  Does  not  match:  will  not  enhance,  just  stop  the  decline   c) Incorrect:  Matches   d) Correct:  Matches   Question  4   a) Incorrect:  True   b) Incorrect:  True   c) Incorrect  :  True   d) Correct:  False:  could  reduce,  not  reduce…(degrees  of  certainty)   Question  5   a) Correct   b) Incorrect   c) Incorrect   d) Incorrect     Question  6   a) Incorrect:  Not  mentioned   b) Incorrect:  True:  but  a  detail   c) Incorrect:  True,  but  not  the  main  idea   d) Correct:  synonym:  unclear=unknown   Question  7   a) Correct:  see  highlight     b) incorrect:     c) Incorrect:  could  be  true  but  not  mentioned   d) Incorrect:     Question  8   a) Incorrect:  no  new  developments  mentioned   b) Incorrect:  not  the  main  focus   c) Correct:  Best  summary   d) Incorrect:  opposite  is  true  

This resource was developed by OET Online and is subject to copyright © Website: http://oetonline.com.au Email: [email protected]

Reading Part B

 

Alzheimer  Disease  

Author:  Margaret  Gatz   Source:  Public  Library  of  Open  Science  

  Paragraph  1   Physicians  now  commonly  advise  older  adults  to  engage  in  mentally  stimulating   activity  as  a  way  of  reducing  their  risk  of  dementia.  Indeed,  (1)the   recommendation  is  often  followed  by  the  acknowledgment  that  evidence  of   benefit  is  still  lacking,  but  “it  can’t  hurt.”  What  could  possibly  be  the  problem   with  older  adults  spending  their  time  doing  crossword  puzzles  and  anagrams,   completing  puzzles,  or  testing  their  reaction  time  on  a  computer?  In  certain   respects,  there  is  no  problem.  Patients  will  probably  improve  at  the  targeted   skills,  and  may  feel  good—particularly  if  the  activity  is  both  challenging  and   successfully  completed.     Paragraph  2   But  can  it  hurt?  Possibly.  There  are  two  ways  that  encouraging  mental  activity   programs  might  do  more  harm  than  good.  First,  (2)  they  can  falsely  raise   expectations.  Second,  individuals  who  do  develop  dementia  might  be  blamed  for   their  condition.  When  heavy  smokers  get  lung  cancer,  they  are  sometimes  seen   as  having  contributed  to  their  own  fates.  People  with  Alzheimer  disease  might   similarly  be  viewed  as  having  brought  it  on  themselves  through  failure  to   exercise  their  brains.     Paragraph  3   There  is  some  evidence  to  support  the  idea  that  mental  exercise  can  improve   one’s  chances  of  escaping  Alzheimer  disease.  Having  more  years  of  education  has   been  shown  to  be  related  to  a  lower  prevalence  of  Alzheimer  disease.  Typically,   the  risk  of  Alzheimer  disease  is  two  to  four  times  higher  in  those  who  have  fewer   years  of  education,  as  compared  to  those  who  have  more  years  of  education.   Other  epidemiological  studies,  although  with  less  consistency,  have  suggested   that  those  who  engage  in  more  leisure  activities  have  a  lower  prevalence  and   incidence  of  Alzheimer  disease.  Additionally,  (3)longitudinal  studies  have  found   that  older  adults  without  dementia  who  participate  in  more  intellectually   challenging  daily  activities  show  less  decline  over  time  on  various  tests  of   cognitive  performance.     Paragraph  4   However,  both  education  and  leisure  activities  are  imperfect  measures  of  mental   exercise.  For  instance,  leisure  activities  represent  a  combination  of  influences.   Not  only  is  there  mental  activation,  but  there  may  also  be  broader  health  effects,   including  stress  reduction  and  improved  vascular  health—  both  of  which  may   contribute  to  reducing  dementia  risk.  (4)It  could  also  be  that  a  third  factor,  such   as  intelligence,  leads  to  greater  levels  of  education  and  more  engagement  in   cognitively  stimulating  activities,  and  independently,  to  lower  risk  of  dementia.   Research  in  Scotland,  for  example,  showed  that  IQ  test  scores  at  age  11  were   predictive  of  future  dementia  risk  .     This resource was developed by OET Online and is subject to copyright © Website: http://oetonline.com.au Email: [email protected]

Reading Part B

Paragraph  5   The  concept  of  cognitive  reserve  is  often  used  to  explain  why  education  and   mental  stimulation  are  beneficial.  The  term  cognitive  reserve  is  sometimes  taken   to  refer  directly  to  brain  size  or  to  synaptic  density  in  the  cortex.  At  other  times,   cognitive  reserve  is  defined  as  the  ability  to  compensate  for  acquired  brain   pathology.  Taken  together,  the  evidence  is  very  suggestive  that  having  greater   cognitive  reserve  is  related  to  a  reduced  risk  of  Alzheimer  disease.  But  the   evidence  that  mental  exercise  can  increase  cognitive  reserve  and  (5)keep   dementia  at  bay  is  weaker.  In  addition,  people  with  greater  cognitive  reserve   may  choose  mentally  stimulating  leisure  activities  and  jobs,  which  makes  is   difficult  to  precisely  determine  whether  mentally  stimulating  activities  alone  can   reduce  dementia  risk.       Paragraph  6   Cognitive  training  has  demonstrable  effects  on  performance,  on  views  of  self,  and   on  brain  function—but  the  results  are  very  specific  to  the  skills  that  are  trained,   and  (6)  it  is  as  yet  entirely  unknown  whether  there  is  any  effect  on  when   or  whether  an  individual  develops  Alzheimer  disease.  Further,  the  types  of  skills   taught  by  practicing  mental  puzzles  may  be  less  helpful  in  everyday  life  than   more  straightforward  techniques,  such  as  concentrating,  or  taking  notes,  or   putting  objects  in  the  same  place  each  time  so  that  they  won’t  be  lost.     Paragraph  7   So  far,  there  is  little  evidence  that  mental  practice  will  help  prevent  the   development  of  dementia.  There  is  better  evidence  that  good  brain  health  is   determined  by  multiple  factors,  that  brain  development  early  in  life  matters,   and  that  genetic  influences  are  of  great  importance  in  accounting  for  individual   differences  in  cognitive  reserve  and  in  explaining  who  develops  Alzheimer   disease  and  who  does  not.  (7)At  least  half  of  the  explanation  for  individual   differences  in  susceptibility  to  Alzheimer  disease  is  genetic,  although  the  genes   involved  have  not  yet  been  completely  discovered.  The  balance  of  the   explanation  lies  in  environmental  influences  and  behavioral  health  practices,   alone  or  in  interaction  with  genetic  factors.  However,  at  this  stage,  there  is  no   convincing  evidence  that  memory  practice  and  other  cognitively  stimulating   activities  are  sufficient  to  prevent  Alzheimer  disease;  it  is  not  just  a  case  of  “use  it   or  lose  it.”    

This resource was developed by OET Online and is subject to copyright © Website: http://oetonline.com.au Email: [email protected]

Reading Part B

Answer  Key     1.  d  2.  b  3.  a  4.  b  5.  a  6.  a  7.b  8.  c  9.  d  10.  a   Question  1   a) Incorrect:  Not  rarely,  often   b) Incorrect:    Not  mentioned   c) Incorrect:  may  be  implicated,  not  has  lead   d) Correct:  (synonym  resurgence=reappearance)  key  word  may   Question  2   a) Incorrect:  global  warming  not  mentioned   b) Correct:  (synonym  disease  epidemics=outbreaks  of  disease)  important  determinant=strong   relationship   c) Incorrect:  Opposite  is  true   d) Incorrect:  True  fact  but  a  detail  not  the  main  idea   Question  3   a) Correct   b) Incorrect     c) Incorrect     d) Incorrect     Question  4   a) Incorrect:  True:  diarrhoea  increased  by  more  than  2-­‐fold  during  an  El  Niño  event   b) Correct:  Not  true  as  the  effects  are  semi-­‐regular/interannual  i.e  between  years  not  yearly   c) Incorrect:  True:  There  is  evidence  of  a  relationship  between  El  Niño  and  the  timing  of  cholera   epidemics  in  Peru  and  Bangladesh   d) Incorrect:  True:  The  onset  of  meningococcal  meningitis  in  Mali  is  associated  with  large-­‐scale   atmospheric  circulation     Question  5   a) Correct:  The  author  is  trying  demonstrate  that  there  is  a  clear  link  between  climate  factors   and  infectious  disease.  Key  words  However  &  synonym:  current=recent   b) Incorrect:  While  this  may  be  true,  it  s  not  the  authors  main  point   c) Incorrect  :  as  in  B   d) Incorrect   Question  6   a) Correct:  (synonym  coincide=correspond)   b) Incorrect:  incorrect  meaning   c) Incorrect:  not  annually,  one  year  ahead   d) Incorrect:  not  given,  it  is  a  better  predictor  than  temperature   Question  7   a) Incorrect   b) Correct   c) Incorrect   d) Incorrect   Question  8   a) Incorrect:  not  constant,  changes  over  time   b) Incorrect:  This  was  a  past  opinion,  not  now   c) Correct:  similar  meaning   d) Incorrect:  Incomplete  information    Question  9   a) Incorrect   b) Incorrect   c) Incorrect   d) Correct   Question  10   a) Correct:  (synonyms  conventional=orthodox  &  may  be  short  comings=may  fail)   b) Incorrect:  no,  deeper  understanding  is  important   c) Incorrect:  Not  the  data,  the  methods   d) Incorrect:  Degrees  of  certainty:  Could  not  will   This resource was developed by OET Online and is subject to copyright © Website: http://oetonline.com.au Email: [email protected]

Reading Part B

Infectious Diseases and Climatic Influences Bernard Cazelles, Simon Hales Paragraph 1 Complex dynamic relationships between humans, pathogens, and the environment lead to the emergence of new diseases and the re-emergence of old ones. Due to concern about the impact of increasing global climate variability and change, many recent studies have focused on relationships between infectious disease and climate. Paragraph 2 (2)Climate can be an important determinant of vector-borne disease epidemics: geographic and seasonal patterns of infectious disease incidence are often, though not always, driven by climate factors. Mosquito- borne diseases, such as malaria, dengue fever, and Ross River virus, typically show strong seasonal and geographic patterns, as do some intestine diseases. These patterns are unsurprising, given the influence of climate on pathogen replication, vector and disease reservoir populations, and human societies. In Sweden, a trend toward milder winters and early spring arrival may be implicated in an increased incidence of tick-borne encephalitis. (1)The recent resurgence of malaria in the East African highlands may be partly explained by increasing temperatures in that region. However, as yet there are (3) relatively few studies showing clear climatic influences on infectious diseases at interannual or longer timescales. Paragraph 3 The (4) semi-regular El Niño climate cycle, centred on the Pacific Ocean, has an important influence on interannual climate patterns in many parts of the world. This makes El Niño an attractive, albeit imperfect, analogue for the effects of global climate change. In Peru, daily admissions for diarrhoea increased by more than 2-fold during an El Niño event, compared with expected trends based on the previous five years. There is evidence of a relationship between El Niño and the timing of cholera epidemics in Peru and Bangladesh; of ciguatera in the Pacific islands; of Ross River virus epidemics in Australia; and of dengue and malaria epidemics in several countries. The onset of meningococcal meningitis in Mali is associated with largescale atmospheric circulation. Paragraph 4 These studies were performed mostly at country scale, reflecting the availability of data sources and, perhaps, the geographically local effects of El Niño on climate. In part because of this geographic “patchiness” of the epidemiological evidence, the identification of climatic factors in infectious disease dynamics, and the relative importance of the different factors, remains controversial. For example, it has been suggested that climate trends are unlikely to contribute to the timing of dengue epidemics in Thailand. (5)However, recent work has shown a strong but transient association between dengue incidence and El Niño in Thailand. This association may possibly be caused by a “pacemaker-like” effect in which intrinsic disease dynamics interact with climate variations driven by El Niño to propagate travelling waves of infection.

This resource was developed by OET Online and is subject to copyright © Website: http://oetonline.com.au Email: [email protected]

Reading Part B

Paragraph 5 A new study on cutaneous leishmaniasis by Chaves and Pascual also provides fresh evidence of a relationship between climate and vector-borne disease. Chaves and Pascual use a range of mathematical tools to illustrate a clear relationship between climatic variables and the dynamics of cutaneous leishmaniasis, a skin infection transmitted by sandflies. (6)In Costa Rica, cutaneous leishmaniasis displays threeyear cycles that coincide with those of El Niño. Chaves and Pascual use this newly demonstrated association to enhance the forecasting ability of their models and to predict the epidemics of leishmaniasis up to one year ahead. Interestingly, El Niño was a better predictor of disease than temperature, possibly because this large-scale index integrates numerous environmental processes and so is a more biologically relevant measure than local temperature. As the authors note, the link between El Niño and epidemics of leishmaniasis might be explained by large-scale climate effects on population susceptibility. Susceptibility, in turn, may be related to lack of specific immunity or poor nutritional status, both of which are plausibly influenced by climate. Paragraph 6 Chaves and Pascual have identified a robust relationship between climate and disease, with changes over time in average incidence and in cyclic components. The dynamics of cutaneous leishmaniasis evolve coherently with climatic variables including temperature and El Niño indices, demonstrating a strong association between these variables, particularly after 1996. (8)Long-term changes in climate, human demography, and social features of human populations have large effects on the dynamics of epidemics as underlined by the analyses of some large datasets on whooping cough and measles. Another illuminating example is the transient relationship between cholera prevalence and El Niño oscillations. In Bangladesh, early in the 20th century, cholera and El Niño appeared unrelated, yet a strong association emerged in 1980–2001. Transient relationships between climate and infectious disease may be caused by interactions between climate and intrinsic disease mechanisms such as temporary immunity. If population susceptibility is low, even large increases in transmission potential due to climate forcing will not result in a large epidemic. Paragraph 7 A deeper understanding of infectious disease dynamics is important in order to forecast, and perhaps forestall, the effects of dramatic global social and environmental changes. (10) Conventional statistical methods may fail to reveal a relationship between climate and health when discontinuous associations are present. Because classical methods quantify average associations over the entire dataset, they may not be adequate to decipher long-term but discontinuous relationships between environmental exposures and human health. On the other hand, relationships between climate and disease could signal problems for disease prediction. Unless all important effects are accounted for, dynamic forecast models may prove to have a limited shelf life. !  

This resource was developed by OET Online and is subject to copyright © Website: http://oetonline.com.au Email: [email protected]

View more...

Comments

Copyright ©2017 KUPDF Inc.
SUPPORT KUPDF